Rajzelemzési Intézet
A hiteles tudás
0.00%
After discussing the most important theoretical issues in the understanding of projective drawings and paintings, this chapter describes how to apply theory into practice. In order to find the true meaning of a pictorial feature (or observations on test behaviour), we need to construct a system. This should be as simple as possible and comprehensive at the same time.
In the previous chapter, the principle of multiple causality showed that the interpretation of a projective product requires the understanding of many interrelated factors. Pictures cannot be understood when they are isolated from their context: they are fragments of the totality of behaviour, and they need to be placed in a broader context.
The picture itself, the personality and present state of the subject, the demographic background, the test instruction, the latent factors of the test situation and the examiner’s personality and attitudes, all construct a network of factors and meanings.
This network is a complex system with a large number of components interrelated in an individual way, so a particular feature of a picture may be the effect of several different causes.
However, this complicated situation can be simplified without losing information. First, we must construct a system of factors. In the first step of the SSCA (context analysis), the examiner explores the contribution and interrelationship of contextual factors in relation to the psychological meaning of the picture. In other words, to understand a picture, one needs to contextualize (to place the picture in an individual context) as a starting point.
To accomplish this aim, the examiner must
As mentioned before, this can be done only from a systems analysis point of view.
There is a practical technique described in chapter 1.2.3 “The phenomenological map” that makes it possible to overview the system in its totality. Ideally, context analysis produces a number of hypotheses that should be checked in the seventh step of the SSCA method. The final result is represented by a semantic map (for details, see chapter 7.2 “The semantic map”).
Context analysis in the SSCA originates from the brilliant works of Wolfgang Sehringer, Professor of Psychology, in Heidelberg. Sehringer’s works are incomparable. His essays and books present many original ideas, including theoretical concepts as the systems analysis approach to children’s drawings (Sehringer, 1989, 1992, 2000).In his systems analysis approach, Sehringer distinguished four components of the interpretation of children’s drawings: the picture itself, the subject, the examiner and the general context. These four components enter into six types of interactions. Using these components, Sehringer described the network of the six interactions and illustrated them with children’s drawings. He emphasised the dynamic changes of the relationships of the components throughout the course of time (instead of in a snap-shot).Due to the dynamics of the system, elements are activated in different ways, therefore, a general model does not work. The examiner must understand the six possible interactions of the four elements in an idiographic way.The SSCA restructured Sehringer’s model in the following aspects: (1) the component “context” was replaced with “situation”, which changed the system’s meaning; (2) each component’s context was defined in detail; (3) the term “contextualization” was added to the model, which referred to the exploration of the context of each component in individual cases; (4) the interactions of the components were redefined as systemic interactions; (5) a fifth component, with a seventh kind of interaction, was introduced; (6) the model’s scope was extended to any pictorial production and (7) the model was adapted to pair, couple or group assessment situations.
Context analysis in the SSCA originates from the brilliant works of Wolfgang Sehringer, Professor of Psychology, in Heidelberg. Sehringer’s works are incomparable. His essays and books present many original ideas, including theoretical concepts as the systems analysis approach to children’s drawings (Sehringer, 1989, 1992, 2000).
In his systems analysis approach, Sehringer distinguished four components of the interpretation of children’s drawings: the picture itself, the subject, the examiner and the general context. These four components enter into six types of interactions. Using these components, Sehringer described the network of the six interactions and illustrated them with children’s drawings. He emphasised the dynamic changes of the relationships of the components throughout the course of time (instead of in a snap-shot).
Due to the dynamics of the system, elements are activated in different ways, therefore, a general model does not work. The examiner must understand the six possible interactions of the four elements in an idiographic way.
The SSCA restructured Sehringer’s model in the following aspects: (1) the component “context” was replaced with “situation”, which changed the system’s meaning; (2) each component’s context was defined in detail; (3) the term “contextualization” was added to the model, which referred to the exploration of the context of each component in individual cases; (4) the interactions of the components were redefined as systemic interactions; (5) a fifth component, with a seventh kind of interaction, was introduced; (6) the model’s scope was extended to any pictorial production and (7) the model was adapted to pair, couple or group assessment situations.
Context analysis summarises all significant factors in seven kinds of bidirectional interactions pertaining to five main components. As Sehringer already stated, the components are the picture itself, the subject, the examiner and the situation (or context, according to Sehringer).
The fifth component is the other person, which is necessary if
The five components enter into seven kinds of bidirectional interactions.
They are listed below in the suggested order of exploring them:
In this way, the model covers all essential factors (components, their context and their interactions). There is no sense in adding more than five components to the model— it remains essentially the same even if there are more than two subjects present in the test situation.
For example, when three subjects draw together, the interaction between the third subject and all the other components can be described in a way similar to the case of the second subject.
The practical significance of the model is that it unfolds the hidden layers of the personality. Without the model, significant contextual factors and conclusions would remain unexplored. It can be used in individual testing, in couple, marital or family therapy, in art therapy or in group examinations.
In the first step of the SSCA, the examiner explores the context of the components, then their interactions (see figure 10). It should be emphasised that exploration is always an idiographic procedure. Each component is a subsystem, consisting of individual factors. Not only are these factors idiographic, but their interactions are also individual and personalised. Some factors and interactions may be of key importance in one case, but irrelevant in another.
Besides the idiographic features of the context of components and their relationships, there are typical ones. A number of typical components and relationships are listed in chapter 1.2.2 “Exploration of systemic interactions” based on previous research (Vass, 2006).
* A four-component version of this diagram was first published by Sehringer (1989).
Következő
Context analysis consists of two steps: (1) contextualization and (2) the exploration of systemic interactions.
The examiner starts with the subject’s psychological, social and physical context.
Starting with the psychological context, the case history is disclosed, but only those facts that are relevant to the picture being examined or to the aim of the assessment. The exploration of case history is generally based on the subject’s self-report (see Appendix 2 for interview questions), or it may be completed with a heteroanamnesis (e.g. with the parents of the child).
During the interview, not only facts and data should be collected, but the examiner should also pay attention to the subject’s personal biases about certain topics, levels and degrees of verbal exaggeration, hidden interests and agendas, assumptions and facts and fictitious opinions or ideas. In the examination of the psychological context, moderator variables should always be carefully reviewed (age, sex, handedness, education, occupation, intelligence, manual dexterity, artistic skills etc.).
The significant factors of social context that may influence the picture’s interpretation are the number and impact of peers, colleagues, friends and the socio-economic status, milieu, culture and subculture of the subject with their shared experiences or ideologies.
The subject’s physical context generally includes present living conditions and current physical state.
The situation influences the picture with its physical, social and psychological context.
Physical context includes the place and time of the examination, as well as the equipment available. Place refers to the location of the assessment (e.g. the workplace, a clinic or hospital, a university and sometimes the home of the subject or the examiner), which may or may not be previously known to the subject.
The choice of place may have an impact on the demand characteristics, the time available and the other factors of the social and psychological context of the assessment.
Also significant are the objects and installations of the room (e.g. the size and position of the table and chairs, especially in couple-, family- and group assessments and in art therapy), the personal objects of the examiner or any pictures visible to the subject (e.g. art therapy objects, drawing tests by other subjects or any other suggestive images).
Environmental stimuli, such as noise, light and temperature, may be also disturbing, neutral or facilitating in terms of pictorial production, i.e. the time of the production (e.g. day of the week, part of the day, season of the year, period of art history etc.), and time context, i.e. the time available for the assessment.
Finally, the physical context includes the equipment available for the creation of the picture (e.g. the size, quality and quantity of the paper, the assortment of pencils, crayons or other tools, even the surface on which the drawing is made).
Social context is defined as the impact of other people on the subject or examiner.
Other people may be present in the assessment situation (e.g. other subjects, other examiners, observers, parents, peers etc.). The situation’s cultural environment also belongs to the social context, for example, the public opinion, attitudes and the preference or avoidance of projective testing among professionals in a given country.
Psychological context includes demand characteristics (cues that make subjects aware of the examiner’s expectations), for example, the labelling of the situation by the examiner (personality test or creativity assessment). It can also be labelled by the subject or by others who are significant to the subject. The wording of the assessment’s introduction may also influence the picture, as well as the suggestions related to the process of creating the picture, the choice of material, the expected drawing time or the quality, detailing, choice of themes and so on.
Another source of demand characteristics is when the examiner asks biased or leading questions.
The situation’s psychological context also includes the instructions (content and wording) and the aim of the testing procedure, which may or may not be the same as the examiner’s and the subject’s objectives.
Since the picture’s physical, social and psychological context generally overlap with the situation’s context, the key factor here is the pictorial context.
This primarily includes other pictures by the same subject, but pictures made by others may also be relevant to the product (see chapter 2.3.1 “The comparison of several pictures”).
Other pictures by the same subject are excellent sources of information. These pictures can be made during the same session or at other times before or after the present picture.
Pictures made at other times should be compared with the present picture in order to explore the similarities and the differences, either in a “miniature longitudinal study” (see chapter 2.3.2 “Comparing pictures made at the same time”) or by using the principles of Hárdi’s (1983, 2002) dynamic examination. Pictorial changes may reflect changes in personality or in the dynamism of defense mechanisms and personality functioning.
Pictures made by others can also be contrasted with the subject’s pictures. The differences in pictures similar to the present picture in theme or in title are always good starting points for the understanding of individuals.
Comparison to the pictures of others also makes it possible to evaluate the picture’s relationship in connection with developmental and other norms (e.g. screening examinations and Koppitz’s developmental and emotional indicators).
The examiner’s psychological context is often a neglected, but crucial factor in the understanding the picture’s psychological meaning.
Empirical data show that the personality characteristics of the examiner, such as age, sex, present mood, psychological state, expertise, practice, theoretical approach and interpretation methods, may influence the conclusion.
For example, Zians (1997) demonstrated that experts and non-experts may draw different conclusions from the same pictures as a result of their different methods.
The adequate method for interpreting pictures requires a careful examination of the examiner’s context through self-reflection and introspection or the supervision of a clinician trained in projective techniques.
Also relevant is the examiner’s cognition, point of view and approach to the interpretation of pictorial expression: a psychoanalytic, developmental or system analytical approach.
For expectations, preconceptions, prejudice and stereotypes see chapter 1.3.2 “Cognitive biases”.
The term “systemic interactions” refers to the interrelationship between components (i.e. subsystems) in the subject-product-examiner-situation system. The system consists of three types of interactions: situational, interpersonal and picture-person (see figure 10).
Systemic interactions should not be confused with pictorial interactions, which denote the semantic relationships between motifs drawn or painted. Pictorial interactions pertain to item analysis, and they are discussed in the sixth step of the SSCA.
This kind of bidirectional interaction refers to the situation’s impact on the subject and the subject’s impact on the situation. It also includes interactions between contextual factors (which were discussed previously).
After exploring the physical, social and psychological context of the subject and the situation, there are some typical questions to be asked. The following questions serve as good starting points and should always be viewed in light of what they reveal about the subject’s psychological functioning:
This includes the situation’s impact on the picture and the picture’s impact on the situation.
The examiner explores how the picture refers to and how it is explained by the impact of the physical, social, psychological and pictorial context in which the picture is created.
Recommended questions are:
Impact of the physical context:
Impact of the social context:
Impact of the psychological context:
Impact of the pictorial context:
This kind of bidirectional interaction refers to the examiner’s influence on the assessment situation and, vice versa, the situation’s influence on the examiner’s behaviour.
To understand these factors, the examiner should reflect on the situation from a broader point of view and have the capacity to exercise self-reflection, self- examination and introspection.
Physical factors:
Psychological factors:
Subject–subject interactions are examined in multi-person situations when more than one subject makes a picture at one time.
This is frequently seen in art therapy (see Waller, 1993), family therapy or couple therapy (conjoint drawings, group drawings and drawing together methods; see Appendix 1).
Although many authors reported the use of multi-person drawing or painting methods (e.g. Elfriede Höhn, 1952; Hanna Kwiatkowska, 1975; Helen Landgarten, 1981), the first comprehensive and systematic method of evaluation was published by a Hungarian research group (see “Drawing together method”: Viola Nagy, 2007, 2008; Viola Vass and Zoltán Vass, 2009).They provided coloured pencils, crayons, felt-tip pens and an A3 sized paper to two subjects and larger sheets for more subjects with the following instruction: “Everyone draw something in 15 minutes, please”. After finishing the picture, they told the subjects to “Give it a title and write it on the picture”.Through 600 video recordings, the authors analysed subject–subject interactions in a number of different populations (e.g. hyperactive and normal children with their mothers, family members, adult couples). As a result of that research, the authors made a distinction between global multi-person evaluations and specific multi-person behavioural reactions. A total number of 61 global behavioural reaction items and 68 specific reaction items were defined and interpreted in an objective coding system.
Although many authors reported the use of multi-person drawing or painting methods (e.g. Elfriede Höhn, 1952; Hanna Kwiatkowska, 1975; Helen Landgarten, 1981), the first comprehensive and systematic method of evaluation was published by a Hungarian research group (see “Drawing together method”: Viola Nagy, 2007, 2008; Viola Vass and Zoltán Vass, 2009).
They provided coloured pencils, crayons, felt-tip pens and an A3 sized paper to two subjects and larger sheets for more subjects with the following instruction: “Everyone draw something in 15 minutes, please”. After finishing the picture, they told the subjects to “Give it a title and write it on the picture”.
Through 600 video recordings, the authors analysed subject–subject interactions in a number of different populations (e.g. hyperactive and normal children with their mothers, family members, adult couples). As a result of that research, the authors made a distinction between global multi-person evaluations and specific multi-person behavioural reactions. A total number of 61 global behavioural reaction items and 68 specific reaction items were defined and interpreted in an objective coding system.
According to Vass and Vass (2009), the analysis of subject–subject interaction consists of (1) a global evaluation, (2) specific reactions, (3) communication analysis and (4) an interaction dynamics approach.
This interaction primarily refers to interaction dynamics, transference and countertransference (the unconscious redirection of feelings from one person to another). The factors may be crucial to the validity of the drawing test or pictorial expression.
The examiner should always consider how these factors influence the meaning conveyed by the picture.
This kind of bidirectional interaction consists of the picture’s impact on the subject (described as “dynamic interactivity” in the SSCA) and the subject’s impact on the picture.
The latter includes the process of creating the picture, the model reactions observed, the post-drawing interview, any spontaneous, verbal comments made about the picture, the title of the picture and any text written on it.
The examiner should consider the following aspects:
The picture–examiner interaction refers to the picture’s impact on the examiner and the examiner’s impact on the picture.
Starting points for disclosing the meaning of the picture are listed below:
Befejezés
Az email címet nem tesszük közzé. A kötelező mezőket * karakterrel jelöltük
Hozzászólás
Név *
E-mail cím *
Honlap
Send this to a friend